Advanced250 min

Omitting words

Lesson content
Omission
• relative clauses
When the relative pronoun is omitted, two nouns/
pronouns can occur next to each other.
The wallet a boy found on the steps has been left in my office. (which)

This can be particularly problematic if a relative clause ends with a preposition.
The role model Jane most looked up to was her mother. (who)
• past participle clauses
The relative pronoun and the auxiliary can sometimes be omitted, making the past participle easily mistaken for the main verb.
People caught hunting will be prosecuted. (who are)
Phones hacked into included those of celebrities and ordinary people. (which were)
that clauses
That can be omitted, making the sentence tricky to disentangle.
Did you know [ ] people are saying [ ] Jill believes [ ]cshe’s going to be sacked?

Subject
We often omit a subject pronoun in spoken language.
Don’t really know.
Haven’t read it, I’m afraid.
Didn’t get to the end of the film.
Must be diffi cult, getting such an early train every morning.

Auxiliary
We can omit the auxiliary in informal questions.
You going to the party later?
They finished the roadworks outside your house yet?

Notice that we don’t omit am.
Am I bringing the sandwiches? NOT I bringing the sandwiches?

Pronoun + auxiliary
We sometimes omit both the pronoun and the auxiliary in spoken language.
Interesting news this morning.
Not seen him today.

Articles
If we are sure who or what we are talking about, we can sometimes omit an article.
Teacher said we’ve got an assignment coming up.
Essay I did last week got full marks.
Book’s really good. You must read it.
Lecture went on for ages!

Quiz

Question 1 of 10

Which word is omitted in the sentence: 'The wallet a boy found on the steps has been left in my office.'

which
that
who
whose

Choices: The Crucible of Human Experience

Every moment of our lives presents us with choices—from the seemingly trivial decision of what to have for breakfast to the life-altering choice of whom to marry. These decisions, large and small, weave together to form the intricate tapestry of human existence, shaping not only our individual destinies but also the collective trajectory of society. In an age of unprecedented complexity and overwhelming options, understanding the psychology and neuroscience behind our decision-making processes has become more crucial than ever. We stand at the crossroads of ancient wisdom and cutting-edge research, where traditional notions of free will intersect with revolutionary insights into the cognitive mechanisms that drive our choices.

The paradox of choice in modern life has reached staggering proportions. Where our ancestors might have faced a handful of life paths, today's individuals navigate an almost infinite maze of possibilities. This explosion of options, while theoretically liberating, has given rise to what researchers call "choice overload"—a phenomenon where too many alternatives can lead to paralysis, regret, and diminished satisfaction with our decisions. Yet within this complexity lies an extraordinary opportunity to understand the fundamental nature of human agency and the intricate dance between reason and emotion that governs our choices.

## The Architecture of Decision-Making: Insights from Cognitive Science

Recent groundbreaking research in cognitive psychology has revolutionized our understanding of how choices are made. The traditional view of decision-making as a purely rational process has been thoroughly debunked by decades of empirical research. Instead, scientists now recognize that our choices emerge from a complex interplay of cognitive systems, each operating according to different principles and timescales.

The dual-process theory, championed by researchers like Daniel Kahneman, distinguishes between two distinct modes of thinking. System 1 operates quickly and automatically, relying on heuristics and intuitive judgments to navigate familiar territory. This system excels at pattern recognition and emotional evaluation but can be prone to systematic biases. System 2, in contrast, engages in deliberate, analytical thinking that requires conscious effort and cognitive resources. While more accurate in many contexts, this system is slower and more demanding, leading us to rely heavily on the rapid-fire assessments of System 1.

Challenge Theory, a recent development in decision science, conceptualizes cognitive decision-making as two sequential thought processes: the heuristic system that reacts to probabilities and defines the default option, and the deliberate system that reevaluates the default and may opt for the "bold" alternative. This framework provides elegant explanations for seemingly irrational behaviors, such as the certainty effect and loss aversion, by recognizing that our brains are fundamentally wired to prioritize survival and conserve cognitive energy.

Neuroscientific investigations have revealed the biological substrate of these processes, showing how different brain regions contribute to various aspects of choice. The prefrontal cortex orchestrates complex decision-making processes, while the limbic system—including the amygdala and nucleus accumbens—processes emotional significance and reward anticipation. The interplay between these systems creates the rich phenomenology of human choice, where logic and feeling interweave in ways that often surprise even the decision-makers themselves.

## The Experience-Choice Nexus: Learning from Life

One of the most significant developments in decision research has been the recognition that choices made from experience differ fundamentally from those based on description. When we make decisions from experience, we rely on accumulated memories, emotional associations, and pattern recognition honed through repeated encounters with similar situations. This experiential learning shapes our future choices in profound ways, creating feedback loops that can either enhance our decision-making abilities or trap us in suboptimal patterns.

Research on decisions from experience has revealed fascinating insights about how we process feedback and update our understanding of choice options. Studies show that we tend to overweight recent experiences while underweighting historical data, a tendency known as recency bias. This bias helps explain why someone might continue gambling after a recent win, despite a long history of losses, or why voters might make decisions based on recent economic performance rather than long-term trends.

The mechanisms underlying experiential learning involve complex interactions between attention, memory, and valuation systems. When we encounter feedback from our choices, the brain doesn't simply store this information neutrally. Instead, it processes the emotional significance of outcomes, updates our expectations about future results, and modifies the neural pathways that guide subsequent decisions. This dynamic process means that our choice architecture is constantly evolving, shaped by the accumulating weight of experience.

Particularly intriguing is the research on counterfactual thinking—our ability to imagine alternative outcomes from choices we didn't make. Recent studies using computational modeling have shown that we actively update our valuations of unchosen options based on observed outcomes, even without direct feedback about those alternatives. This capacity for mental simulation allows us to learn vicariously and expand our decision-making repertoire beyond direct experience.

## The Emotional Landscape of Choice

Traditional economic models assumed that emotions were merely noise in the decision-making process—irrational influences to be minimized in favor of cold calculation. However, contemporary research has revealed that emotions are not just relevant to choice; they are absolutely essential. Far from being disruptive forces, emotions serve as sophisticated information-processing systems that help us navigate complex social and environmental challenges.

The somatic marker hypothesis, proposed by neuroscientist Antonio Damasio, suggests that emotional responses to potential outcomes guide our decision-making before conscious deliberation even begins. When we contemplate a choice, our bodies generate subtle physiological responses—somatic markers—that reflect our accumulated emotional learning about similar situations. These bodily signals help us rapidly eliminate poor options and focus attention on promising alternatives.

Emotional intelligence plays a crucial role in effective decision-making, particularly in social contexts. Leaders with high emotional intelligence are better able to consider multiple perspectives, anticipate the emotional impact of their choices, and navigate the complex interpersonal dynamics that influence many important decisions. This capacity for emotional awareness and regulation doesn't diminish rational thinking; rather, it enhances it by providing richer information about the human dimensions of choice.

The relationship between emotion and choice becomes particularly complex when we consider the role of mood and affect. Research has shown that our current emotional state can significantly influence the decisions we make, sometimes in ways that seem unrelated to the actual choice at hand. Positive moods tend to promote risk-taking and optimistic assessments, while negative moods often lead to more conservative choices and careful evaluation of potential losses.

## Cognitive Biases: The Double-Edged Sword of Mental Shortcuts

The human mind's capacity for processing information is fundamentally limited, forcing us to rely on cognitive shortcuts—heuristics—to navigate the overwhelming complexity of modern life. These mental tools are remarkably effective in many situations, allowing us to make rapid decisions with minimal cognitive effort. However, they can also lead us astray when applied inappropriately or when the environment differs significantly from the conditions in which they evolved.

Confirmation bias represents one of the most pervasive influences on choice behavior. We naturally seek information that confirms our existing beliefs while avoiding or discounting contradictory evidence. This tendency helps maintain cognitive consistency and reduces the mental effort required to process complex information, but it can also trap us in echo chambers and prevent us from making optimal decisions when circumstances change.

Anchoring effects demonstrate how arbitrary starting points can profoundly influence our evaluations and choices. When we encounter an initial piece of information—the anchor—subsequent judgments become biased toward that reference point, even when the anchor is completely irrelevant to the decision at hand. This phenomenon reveals the fundamentally relative nature of human judgment and the importance of framing in choice architecture.

Loss aversion, perhaps the most robust finding in behavioral economics, shows that losses loom larger than equivalent gains in our psychological evaluation. This asymmetry influences everything from financial decisions to career choices, often leading us to maintain suboptimal status quo situations rather than risk potential losses for potentially greater gains. Understanding this bias is crucial for anyone seeking to make more effective choices or design choice environments that promote better outcomes.

The availability heuristic leads us to judge the probability of events based on how easily we can recall similar instances. This mental shortcut generally serves us well, as more frequent events are indeed easier to remember. However, it can lead to systematic errors when media coverage, personal experience, or emotional salience makes certain types of events more memorable than their actual frequency would warrant.

## The Paradox of Choice in Modern Society

The exponential increase in available options has fundamentally transformed the landscape of human choice. Barry Schwartz's seminal work on the paradox of choice revealed that more options don't always lead to better outcomes or greater satisfaction. Instead, choice proliferation can lead to decision paralysis, increased regret, and opportunity cost rumination that diminishes our enjoyment of selected options.

This phenomenon has particularly profound implications in our digital age, where virtually unlimited options are available at the touch of a screen. Online shopping platforms offer millions of products, streaming services provide thousands of entertainment options, and social media connects us to billions of potential relationships. The sheer volume of choices can overwhelm our cognitive capacity and leave us feeling anxious and dissatisfied regardless of what we ultimately choose.

Research has identified two distinct cognitive styles that influence how people respond to multiple options: maximizers and satisficers. Maximizers seek the best possible option and are willing to invest significant time and effort in exhaustive comparison. While this approach can lead to objectively better outcomes, maximizers often experience greater regret and lower satisfaction with their choices. Satisficers, in contrast, seek options that meet their criteria for "good enough" and stop searching once they find an acceptable alternative. This approach tends to produce higher satisfaction levels, even when the chosen option may not be objectively optimal.

The proliferation of choice has also created new forms of social pressure and status anxiety. When every decision becomes a statement about our identity and values, the stakes of choice-making rise dramatically. The clothes we wear, the food we eat, the entertainment we consume—all become signals that we worry might be scrutinized and judged by others. This performance anxiety can transform routine decisions into sources of stress and self-doubt.

## Choice Architecture and Behavioral Nudges

The recognition that choice environments significantly influence decision outcomes has led to the development of choice architecture—the deliberate design of contexts in which people make decisions. This field, popularized by Richard Thaler and Cass Sunstein, recognizes that there is no such thing as neutral choice presentation. Every decision environment nudges choosers toward particular options through factors like default settings, option ordering, and framing effects.

Behavioral nudges leverage our understanding of cognitive biases and heuristics to guide people toward choices that better serve their long-term interests. Unlike mandates or restrictions, nudges preserve freedom of choice while making beneficial options more appealing or accessible. For example, automatically enrolling employees in retirement savings plans (while allowing them to opt out) dramatically increases participation rates compared to opt-in systems.

The ethical implications of choice architecture are complex and contested. While well-designed nudges can help people make choices that align with their stated values and long-term goals, they also raise questions about paternalism and manipulation. Who decides what constitutes a beneficial outcome? How do we balance individual autonomy with collective welfare? These questions become particularly acute when powerful institutions—governments, corporations, tech platforms—possess sophisticated tools for influencing choice behavior.

The digital revolution has amplified both the opportunities and risks associated with choice architecture. Algorithmic recommendation systems can help us navigate information overload and discover options we might never have found otherwise. However, these same systems can create filter bubbles, exploit psychological vulnerabilities, and prioritize engagement over well-being. The challenge lies in developing choice architectures that genuinely serve human flourishing rather than merely capturing attention or driving consumption.

## The Neuroscience of Free Will and Determinism

Perhaps no aspect of choice research has generated more philosophical controversy than investigations into the neural basis of decision-making. Benjamin Libet's famous experiments in the 1980s showed that brain activity associated with simple choices begins several hundred milliseconds before people report being aware of their intention to act. These findings sparked intense debates about the nature of free will and the relationship between consciousness and choice.

Subsequent research has revealed an even more complex picture of how choices emerge from neural activity. Rather than discrete decision moments, neuroscientists now understand choice as emerging from dynamic competition between different neural populations, each representing different possible actions or outcomes. The "winner" of this competition determines our eventual choice, but the process involves multiple influences, feedback loops, and potential intervention points.

Recent studies have also shown that we can influence our future choices through meditation, cognitive training, and environmental modifications that strengthen certain neural pathways while weakening others. This neuroplasticity suggests that while our choices may be determined by prior neural states, we can play an active role in shaping those determining factors through deliberate practice and environmental design.

The implications of neuroscientific findings for personal responsibility and moral judgment remain hotly debated. Some argue that evidence of neural determinism undermines traditional notions of accountability and desert. Others contend that understanding the mechanisms of choice can actually enhance our capacity for moral agency by revealing intervention points where we can influence our future decision-making patterns.

## Cultural and Individual Variations in Choice Patterns

The psychology of choice is not universal but varies significantly across cultures and individuals. Research has revealed systematic differences in how people from different cultural backgrounds approach decisions, value autonomy versus interdependence, and weigh individual versus collective outcomes. These variations reflect deep-seated differences in worldview, social organization, and value systems that shape choice patterns from early childhood.

Western cultures, particularly those influenced by Enlightenment thinking, tend to emphasize individual autonomy and personal responsibility in choice-making. This cultural framework treats choices as expressions of authentic selfhood and values the freedom to pursue individual preferences. In contrast, many non-Western cultures prioritize social harmony, family obligations, and collective welfare in decision-making, sometimes viewing excessive individual choice as selfish or destabilizing.

Individual differences in personality, cognitive style, and life experience also create substantial variation in choice patterns. Some people naturally gravitate toward risk and novelty, while others prefer security and familiarity. These preferences aren't just matters of taste; they reflect underlying differences in neural sensitivity to rewards and punishments, genetic variations in neurotransmitter systems, and learned associations developed through life experience.

Understanding these variations is crucial for designing effective choice architectures and making sense of decision-making in diverse contexts. What works as a nudge in one cultural context may backfire in another. What seems like an irrational choice from one perspective may be perfectly sensible from a different value framework or life situation.

## The Future of Choice: Technology, Artificial Intelligence, and Human Agency

As artificial intelligence becomes increasingly sophisticated, we face unprecedented questions about the future of human choice. AI systems can already predict our preferences with remarkable accuracy, sometimes knowing what we want before we're consciously aware of it ourselves. This predictive power raises profound questions about the nature of autonomy and the value of human choice in an age of algorithmic optimization.

Machine learning systems trained on vast datasets of human behavior can identify patterns and correlations that escape conscious awareness. They can predict which products we'll buy, which content we'll engage with, and even which romantic partners we're likely to find attractive. This capability offers tremendous benefits in terms of personalization and efficiency, but it also challenges traditional notions of authentic choice and self-determination.

The integration of AI into choice architecture represents both an opportunity and a threat. On one hand, AI can help us overcome cognitive limitations, reduce bias, and make more informed decisions by processing vastly more information than any human could consider. On the other hand, it could lead to a kind of choice atrophy, where we become dependent on algorithmic recommendations and lose the capacity for independent judgment.

Perhaps most intriguingly, AI systems themselves face choice-like problems in their optimization processes. As these systems become more sophisticated, questions about their decision-making mechanisms become increasingly relevant to our understanding of choice itself. Do artificial agents make choices in any meaningful sense? Can they develop preferences, exhibit biases, or demonstrate creativity in their decision-making? These questions may ultimately shed new light on the nature of human choice by providing alternative models of how intelligent systems can navigate complex decision spaces.

## Toward Wiser Choices: Implications for Living Well

Understanding the psychology and neuroscience of choice doesn't eliminate the fundamental challenge of decision-making, but it does provide valuable tools for navigating life's complexities more skillfully. Perhaps most importantly, this research reveals that good decision-making is itself a learnable skill that can be improved through practice, reflection, and environmental design.

The key insight is that choice quality depends not just on analytical prowess but on the integration of multiple sources of information: logical analysis, emotional wisdom, social awareness, and experiential learning. The most effective decision-makers don't try to eliminate emotion or intuition from their choices; instead, they learn to harness these capabilities alongside rational analysis.

Practical strategies emerging from choice research include: diversifying our information sources to combat confirmation bias; implementing waiting periods for important decisions to allow System 2 thinking to engage; seeking out dissenting opinions and steel-manning opposing viewpoints; and designing personal choice architectures that make beneficial options easier and more appealing.

Perhaps most fundamentally, choice research reminds us that our decisions shape not only our outcomes but also our future capacity for making good choices. Each choice we make strengthens certain neural pathways and weakens others, creating feedback loops that influence all subsequent decisions. This understanding places a profound responsibility on us to choose not just for immediate outcomes but for the kind of people we're becoming through our choices.

In a world of overwhelming options and accelerating change, the ability to make wise choices becomes ever more crucial. By understanding the mechanisms that drive our decisions, we can work with our cognitive architecture rather than against it, creating lives that reflect our deepest values and highest aspirations. The choices we make today will determine not only our individual futures but also the collective trajectory of our species as we navigate the challenges and opportunities of an uncertain but boundless future.

Be careful! Omitting words can lead to confusion, especially in writing or in formal situations. Use omission mainly in informal spoken English or when the meaning is very clear from context.

1. seemingly trivial
apparently unimportant

2. life-altering choice
decision that changes everything

3. intricate /ˈɪntrəkət/ US /ˈɪntrɪkət/ UK
very complex

4. tapestry /ˈtæpəstri/ US /ˈtæpəstri/ UK
woven fabric or complex combination

5. individual destinies
personal fates

6. collective trajectory
group path

7. unprecedented /ʌnˈpresəˌdentəd/ US /ʌnˈpresɪˌdentɪd/ UK
never seen before

8. overwhelming options
too many choices

9. decision-making processes
choice procedures

10. stand at the crossroads
be at a point of decision

11. cutting-edge research
most advanced studies

12. traditional notions
old-fashioned ideas

13. free will
ability to choose freely

14. intersect /ˌɪntərˈsekt/ US /ˌɪntəˈsekt/ UK
meet or cross

15. revolutionary /ˌrevəˈluʃəˌneri/ US /ˌrevəˈluːʃənəri/ UK
completely new

16. insights /ˈɪnˌsaɪts/ US /ˈɪnˌsaɪts/ UK
understanding

17. cognitive /ˈkɑɡnətɪv/ US /ˈkɒɡnətɪv/ UK
relating to thinking

18. mechanisms /ˈmekəˌnɪzəmz/ US /ˈmekənɪzəmz/ UK
processes

19. paradox /ˈpærəˌdɑks/ US /ˈpærəˌdɒks/ UK
contradiction

20. staggering proportions
shocking sizes

21. ancestors /ˈænsestərz/ US /ˈænsestəz/ UK
past family members

22. handful of life paths
few life choices

23. navigate /ˈnævəˌɡeɪt/ US /ˈnævɪˌɡeɪt/ UK
find way through

24. infinite maze
endless puzzle

25. explosion of options
huge increase in choices

26. theoretically /ˌθiəˈretɪkli/ US /ˌθɪəˈretɪkli/ UK
in theory

27. liberating /ˈlɪbəˌreɪtɪŋ/ US /ˈlɪbəˌreɪtɪŋ/ UK
freeing

28. choice overload
too many options

29. paralysis /pəˈræləsəs/ US /pəˈræləsɪs/ UK
inability to move or act

30. regret /rɪˈɡret/ US /rɪˈɡret/ UK
feeling sorry about

31. diminished /dɪˈmɪnɪʃt/ US /dɪˈmɪnɪʃt/ UK
reduced

32. satisfaction /ˌsætəsˈfækʃən/ US /ˌsætɪsˈfækʃən/ UK
contentment

33. extraordinary /ɪkˈstrɔrdəˌneri/ US /ɪkˈstrɔːdənəri/ UK
exceptional

34. fundamental nature
basic character

35. human agency
people's power to act

36. intricate dance
complex interaction

37. reason and emotion
logic and feelings

38. governs /ˈɡʌvərnz/ US /ˈɡʌvənz/ UK
controls

39. architecture /ˈɑrkəˌtektʃər/ US /ˈɑːkɪˌtektʃə/ UK
structure

40. groundbreaking research
pioneering studies

41. revolutionized /ˌrevəˈluʃəˌnaɪzd/ US /ˌrevəˈluːʃəˌnaɪzd/ UK
completely changed

42. purely rational process
completely logical method

43. thoroughly debunked
completely disproven

44. empirical /ɪmˈpɪrəkəl/ US /ɪmˈpɪrɪkəl/ UK
based on observation

45. emerge from
come out of

46. complex interplay
complicated interaction

47. cognitive systems
thinking processes

48. operating /ˈɑpəˌreɪtɪŋ/ US /ˈɒpəˌreɪtɪŋ/ UK
working

49. principles /ˈprɪnsəpəlz/ US /ˈprɪnsəpəlz/ UK
basic rules

50. timescales /ˈtaɪmˌskeɪlz/ US /ˈtaɪmˌskeɪlz/ UK
time periods

51. dual-process theory
two-system model

52. championed /ˈtʃæmpjənd/ US /ˈtʃæmpɪənd/ UK
supported

53. distinguishes /dɪˈstɪŋɡwɪʃəz/ US /dɪˈstɪŋɡwɪʃɪz/ UK
tells apart

54. distinct modes
different ways

55. operates quickly
works fast

56. automatically /ˌɔtəˈmætɪkli/ US /ˌɔːtəˈmætɪkli/ UK
without thinking

57. relying on heuristics
using mental shortcuts

58. intuitive /ɪnˈtuətɪv/ US /ɪnˈtjuːətɪv/ UK
instinctive

59. judgments /ˈdʒʌdʒmənts/ US /ˈdʒʌdʒmənts/ UK
decisions

60. navigate /ˈnævəˌɡeɪt/ US /ˈnævɪˌɡeɪt/ UK
find way through

61. familiar territory
known areas

62. excels /ɪkˈselz/ US /ɪkˈselz/ UK
does very well

63. pattern recognition
seeing similarities

64. emotional evaluation
feeling-based assessment

65. prone to
likely to have

66. systematic biases
regular errors

67. engages in
takes part in

68. deliberate /dɪˈlɪbərət/ US /dɪˈlɪbərət/ UK
careful

69. analytical /ˌænəˈlɪtɪkəl/ US /ˌænəˈlɪtɪkəl/ UK
using analysis

70. conscious effort
aware trying

71. cognitive resources
mental energy

72. more demanding
requiring more effort

73. rely heavily
depend greatly

74. rapid-fire assessments
quick evaluations

75. conceptualizes /kənˈseptʃuəˌlaɪzəz/ US /kənˈseptʃʊəˌlaɪzəz/ UK
thinks of as

76. sequential /sɪˈkwenʃəl/ US /sɪˈkwenʃəl/ UK
following order

77. thought processes
thinking methods

78. heuristic /hjʊˈrɪstɪk/ US /hjʊˈrɪstɪk/ UK
mental shortcut

79. reacts to probabilities
responds to chances

80. defines /dɪˈfaɪnz/ US /dɪˈfaɪnz/ UK
sets

81. default option
automatic choice

82. deliberate /dɪˈlɪbərət/ US /dɪˈlɪbərət/ UK
careful

83. reevaluates /ˌriɪˈvæljuˌeɪts/ US /ˌriːɪˈvæljʊˌeɪts/ UK
considers again

84. opt for
choose

85. bold alternative
brave different choice

86. framework /ˈfreɪmˌwɜrk/ US /ˈfreɪmˌwɜːk/ UK
structure

87. elegant explanations
simple beautiful reasons

88. seemingly irrational
apparently unreasonable

89. behaviors /bɪˈheɪvjərz/ US /bɪˈheɪvjəz/ UK
actions

90. certainty effect
preference for sure things

91. loss aversion
dislike of losing

92. recognizing /ˈrekəɡˌnaɪzɪŋ/ US /ˈrekəɡˌnaɪzɪŋ/ UK
understanding

93. fundamentally wired
basically connected

94. prioritize /praɪˈɔrəˌtaɪz/ US /praɪˈɒrəˌtaɪz/ UK
put first

95. survival /sərˈvaɪvəl/ US /səˈvaɪvəl/ UK
staying alive

96. conserve /kənˈsɜrv/ US /kənˈsɜːv/ UK
save

97. cognitive energy
mental power

98. neuroscientific /ˌnʊroʊsaɪənˈtɪfɪk/ US /ˌnjʊərəʊsaɪənˈtɪfɪk/ UK
brain science

99. investigations /ɪnˌvestəˈɡeɪʃənz/ US /ɪnˌvestɪˈɡeɪʃənz/ UK
studies

100. revealed /rɪˈvild/ US /rɪˈviːld/ UK
showed

101. biological substrate
physical basis

102. brain regions
parts of brain

103. contribute to
help with

104. aspects /ˈæspekts/ US /ˈæspekts/ UK
parts

105. prefrontal cortex
front brain area

106. orchestrates /ˈɔrkəˌstreɪts/ US /ˈɔːkɪˌstreɪts/ UK
coordinates

107. complex decision-making
difficult choosing

108. limbic system
emotional brain parts

109. amygdala /əˈmɪɡdələ/ US /əˈmɪɡdələ/ UK
fear center

110. nucleus accumbens
reward center

111. processes /ˈprɑsesəz/ US /ˈprəʊsesɪz/ UK
handles

112. emotional significance
feeling importance

113. reward anticipation
expecting good things

114. interplay /ˈɪntərˌpleɪ/ US /ˈɪntəˌpleɪ/ UK
interaction

115. creates /kriˈeɪts/ US /kriˈeɪts/ UK
makes

116. rich phenomenology
complex experience

117. human choice
people's decisions

118. logic and feeling
reason and emotion

119. interweave /ˌɪntərˈwiv/ US /ˌɪntəˈwiːv/ UK
combine

120. surprise /sərˈpraɪz/ US /səˈpraɪz/ UK
shock

121. decision-makers
people who choose

122. experience-choice nexus
connection between experience and decisions

123. recognition /ˌrekəɡˈnɪʃən/ US /ˌrekəɡˈnɪʃən/ UK
understanding

124. choices made from experience
decisions based on past events

125. differ fundamentally
are basically different

126. based on description
using written information

127. accumulated /əˈkjumjəˌleɪtəd/ US /əˈkjuːmjʊˌleɪtɪd/ UK
gathered

128. memories /ˈmeməriz/ US /ˈmeməriz/ UK
remembrances

129. emotional associations
feeling connections

130. pattern recognition
seeing similarities

131. honed /hoʊnd/ US /həʊnd/ UK
sharpened

132. repeated encounters
happening again

133. similar situations
like circumstances

134. experiential /ɪkˌspɪriˈenʃəl/ US /ɪkˌspɪərɪˈenʃəl/ UK
based on experience

135. shapes /ʃeɪps/ US /ʃeɪps/ UK
influences

136. future choices
later decisions

137. profound ways
deep methods

138. feedback loops
circular effects

139. enhance /ɪnˈhæns/ US /ɪnˈhɑːns/ UK
improve

140. abilities /əˈbɪlətiz/ US /əˈbɪlətiːz/ UK
skills

141. trap us
catch us

142. suboptimal /ˌsʌbˈɑptəməl/ US /ˌsʌbˈɒptɪməl/ UK
not best

143. patterns /ˈpætərnz/ US /ˈpætənz/ UK
repeated designs

144. revealed /rɪˈvild/ US /rɪˈviːld/ UK
shown

145. fascinating /ˈfæsəˌneɪtɪŋ/ US /ˈfæsɪˌneɪtɪŋ/ UK
very interesting

146. insights /ˈɪnˌsaɪts/ US /ˈɪnˌsaɪts/ UK
understanding

147. process /ˈprɑses/ US /ˈprəʊses/ UK
handle

148. feedback /ˈfidˌbæk/ US /ˈfiːdˌbæk/ UK
response

149. update /ˈʌpˌdeɪt/ US /ˌʌpˈdeɪt/ UK
change

150. understanding /ˌʌndərˈstændɪŋ/ US /ˌʌndəˈstændɪŋ/ UK
knowledge

151. choice options
possible decisions

152. tend to overweight
usually give too much importance

153. recent experiences
new events

154. underweighting /ˌʌndərˈweɪtɪŋ/ US /ˌʌndəˈweɪtɪŋ/ UK
not giving enough importance

155. historical data
past information

156. tendency /ˈtendənsi/ US /ˈtendənsi/ UK
inclination

157. recency bias
preference for recent events

158. helps explain
makes clear

159. continue gambling
keep betting

160. recent win
new victory

161. despite /dɪˈspaɪt/ US /dɪˈspaɪt/ UK
in spite of

162. long history
extended past

163. losses /ˈlɔsəz/ US /ˈlɒsɪz/ UK
defeats

164. voters /ˈvoʊtərz/ US /ˈvəʊtəz/ UK
people who vote

165. economic performance
financial results

166. long-term trends
extended patterns

167. mechanisms /ˈmekəˌnɪzəmz/ US /ˈmekənɪzəmz/ UK
processes

168. underlying /ˌʌndərˈlaɪɪŋ/ US /ˌʌndəˈlaɪɪŋ/ UK
basic

169. experiential learning
experience-based education

170. involve /ɪnˈvɑlv/ US /ɪnˈvɒlv/ UK
include

171. complex interactions
complicated connections

172. attention /əˈtenʃən/ US /əˈtenʃən/ UK
focus

173. memory /ˈmeməri/ US /ˈmeməri/ UK
remembering

174. valuation /ˌvæljuˈeɪʃən/ US /ˌvæljʊˈeɪʃən/ UK
value assessment

175. systems /ˈsɪstəmz/ US /ˈsɪstəmz/ UK
organized methods

176. encounter /ɪnˈkaʊntər/ US /ɪnˈkaʊntə/ UK
meet

177. feedback /ˈfidˌbæk/ US /ˈfiːdˌbæk/ UK
response

178. brain /breɪn/ US /breɪn/ UK
thinking organ

179. store /stɔr/ US /stɔː/ UK
keep

180. information /ˌɪnfərˈmeɪʃən/ US /ˌɪnfəˈmeɪʃən/ UK
data

181. neutrally /ˈnutrəli/ US /ˈnjuːtrəli/ UK
without bias

182. processes /ˈprɑsesəz/ US /ˈprəʊsesɪz/ UK
handles

183. emotional significance
feeling importance

184. outcomes /ˈaʊtˌkʌmz/ US /ˈaʊtˌkʌmz/ UK
results

185. updates /ˈʌpˌdeɪts/ US /ˌʌpˈdeɪts/ UK
changes

186. expectations /ˌekspekˈteɪʃənz/ US /ˌekspekˈteɪʃənz/ UK
hopes

187. future results
later outcomes

188. modifies /ˈmɑdəˌfaɪz/ US /ˈmɒdɪˌfaɪz/ UK
changes

189. neural pathways
brain connections

190. guide /ɡaɪd/ US /ɡaɪd/ UK
direct

191. subsequent /ˈsʌbsəkwənt/ US /ˈsʌbsɪkwənt/ UK
following

192. dynamic process
changing method

193. choice architecture
decision structure

194. constantly evolving
always changing

195. shaped by
influenced by

196. accumulating /əˈkjumjəˌleɪtɪŋ/ US /əˈkjuːmjʊˌleɪtɪŋ/ UK
building up

197. weight of experience
importance of past events

198. particularly intriguing
especially interesting

199. research on
studies about

200. counterfactual thinking
imagining alternatives